Omega Statistics Blog


Propensity Scores vs. Regression Adjustment for Observational Studies

Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are considered the gold standard approach for estimating treatment effects. However, not all clinical research involves randomization of subjects into treatment and control groups. These studies are commonly referred to as non-experimental or observational studies. Some examples of non-experimental (observational) studies include:

  • Comparing a treatment group with a group of historical controls
  • Subjects pick the treatment they desire, hence, they self-select into a particular group
  • Subjects are compared on a variable that cannot be randomized, such as gender, race/ethnicity, drug use (yes/no)
  • Subjects are retrospectively pulled from a large dataset for review.
  • In observational studies, the treatment selection is influenced by the characteristics of subjects. Therefore, any differences between groups are not randomized out, and the baseline characteristics of the subjects could differ between treatment and control groups. In essence, the baseline characteristics systematically differ and we must find a way to account for these systematic baseline differences.

    Researchers often use regression adjustments to account for baseline differences between groups. A regression adjustment is made by using one or more (usually more) variables obtained at baseline as predictors, and one dependent variable of the treatment outcome. Using a regression adjustment to investigate baseline differences in observational studies has the following issues:

  • It is difficult to determine whether the model specification in a regression is the correct one to use. A researcher cannot reliably measure whether the variables he or she chooses are indeed the correct ones to use to control for the systematic baseline differences between groups. Model diagnostics such as the model R-squared of a multiple linear regression gives an indication of how well the predictors “predict” the outcome, but knowing how well a model fits as it relates to an outcome doesn’t tell us whether the model chosen actually included the predictors related to systematic baseline differences.
  • Using a regression model with the treatment as an outcome introduces researcher bias. This is because it can be very tempting for researchers to try different model specifications to get the model they desire. For instance, a researcher might, in good faith, start with a model that includes baseline variables that he or she believes are different between groups. Then, when the findings of the regression model indicate no significant effect on the outcome, or the model R-squared is too low, the researcher will change or add predictors to enhance the model. Not a good idea. And as noted above, it is very tempting to do.
  • Propensity scores, and matching subjects from each of the study groups using propensity scores, are constructed without taking the treatment outcome into consideration. The use of propensity scores keeps the researcher’s attention on baseline characteristics only. However, once the subjects are scored and matched (defined as balanced), a regression model can be analyzed to further adjust for any residual imbalance between the groups. So regression models still have their use! But they are used after the propensity scoring and matching.

    Propensity scores have the added benefit of allowing a researcher to see the actual amount of overlap, or lack of it, between treatment groups. After propensity scores are assigned to each individual in each group, then the researcher uses the scores to “match” pairs of subjects in the treatment with subjects in the control group. The easiest way to match is with a one-to-one match: one treatment subject to one control subject. But some, more-advanced matching techniques can match one-to-many.

    After matching, the researcher can see if there are many unmatched individuals left over (indicative of large differences in baseline characteristics between the groups). A large difference between groups might not just indicate that the treatment and control groups differed at baseline, but that the differences between groups might be too large to assess any meaning on the outcome of the intervention. After all, the reason for propensity score matching is to derive groups that simulate equal baseline characteristics. If they can’t be matched, they were just not similar. Hence, treatment efficacy cannot be derived or established.

    Here is a very simple example of the use of propensity scores and matching for a non-experimental study:

    A study is performed to assess the treatment effects of two analgesic drugs given to patients presenting to an emergency room with severe cluster headaches. The type of drug given, A or B, is decided upon various factors such as the time span of the current headache, frequency of headaches, age of the patient, and various comorbidities. Thus, the patients were not randomized into the two drug treaments.

    This non-randomization is also called “selection bias”. The clinician selected the treatment to give to each patient. If more than one clinician was involved in the decision making of treatment, we should control for this also! Perhaps some clinicians like one treatment over the other.

    The outcome is time to pain management. And looking at the data without any adjustment, Drug A appears to relive pain significantly faster than Drug B. But, maybe this is not the case. Something else could be at work here, or maybe there isn’t a difference between the drugs at all. Or maybe the patients in Group A (patients who took drug A) are much too different from the patients in Group B (patients who took Drug B) to make any assessment of efficacy.

    So, we will develop a propensity score for each patient based on the covariates we believe (or better, know from our knowledge and the literature). The propensity score, let’s call it Z, predicts how likely a person is to get Drug A.

  • We assume that the likelihood of a person receiving Drug A is very similar for all people with the same propensity score Z.
  • We then group people with similar propensity scores between the two groups of patients, such that patients with, say, Z = .30 in Group A are matched with patients with Z = .30 in Group B.

  • Then we can run tests on matched groups of patients to test treatment efficacy. With propensity score matching, we’ve removed some of the effects of baseline differences, and now we have something close to tiny RCT’s.

    Nothing is as good as the RCT, but I hope I’ve opened up your thinking a bit to the use of propensity scores in observational studies. There are many ways to score and match and analyze observational study groups. A good reference to start with is the article by Austin (2011) listed below. Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983) wrote the seminal work on propensity scores, but even I think it is a bit too theory heavy. But it is also listed in the references below if you are so inclined.

    Not everyone likes propensity scores for matching cases. The article by King and Nielsen (2016, also referenced below) presents some limitations in propensity score matching and some remedies for when many individual cases remain after the matching attempt.
    Stata has a function for tseffects for obtaining propensity scores, and the function of psmatch for propensity score matching. You can also run post-estimation regression with the functions.

    For R fans, here is a nice tutorial on propensity score matching. Not as nice as the Stata code, but hey, it’s free! http://pareonline.net/pdf/v19n18.pdf

    References

    Austin, Peter. (2011). An Introduction to Propensity Score Methods for Reducing the Effects of Confounding in Observational Studies. Multivariate behavioral research. 46. 399-424. 10.1080/00273171.2011.568786.

    R. Rosenbaum, Paul & B Rubin, David. (1983). The Central Role of the Propensity Score in Observational Studies for Causal Effects. Biometrika. 70. 41-55. 10.2307/2335942.

    Not everyone likes propensity scores for matching:
    Gary King and Richard Nielsen. Working Paper. “Why Propensity Scores Should Not Be Used for Matching”. Copy at http://j.mp/2ovYGsW

  • Join the Stats for the Masses mailing list to receive the “Stats Grab Bag” email with useful resources and links relating to the work I do each week. I like to think of it as tidbits from practice. You’ll never know what you’ll get, but it could be just what you need!

    JOIN THE MAILING LIST


    Thank You - Watch Your Inbox For Our Next News Letter

    Accreditations and Memberships


    Murrieta Chamber Of Commerce Logo
    Better Business Bureau Logo
    American Statistical Association